Category Size Bias

We're more influenced by options that are put into a relatively-large group.

Recent research has shown that we feel that outcomes classified into large categories are more likely to occur than outcomes classified into small categories. Turns out that this is because we incorrectly think that individual outcomes take on properties of their overall category.

Isaac & Brough (2014) Judging a Part by the Size of Its Whole: The Category Size Bias in Probability Judgments. Journal of Consumer Research.

In brief

  1. The findings from this braingem can nudge better healthcare choices, encourage consumption of a given product, and lead to more confident consumer decisions.
  2. We mistakenly believe that items in larger categories have a higher probability of being picked than ones in smaller categories, despite all items having an equal chance of being picked.
  3. We’ll spend or gamble more money on items put in larger categories.
  4. We’re more likely to take action from tasks when they’re in a bigger list, over a smaller list.
  5. We once we put something into a group, we perceive it to adopt all the characteristics of that group. This suggests that small companies should foster alliances with similarly-principled, more established companies.

The roulette wheel

Imagine for a moment that you’re in a casino, standing at the roulette wheel. With one ball in the croupier’s hand, you’ve one chance to win BIG…

On the wheel, there are 18 black pockets but only 2 green ones. Now, how much more likely are you to land a specific black pocket over a green one?

Despite the fact that we’ve a completely equal chance of landing on either, many make the mistake of thinking that success will be more likely with the black choice than the green. This is because there are more black pockets (18) than green ones (2). Our judgment on the probability of landing on a ‘Black two’, for instance, is affected by the category it sits within. Black’s a bigger category than green, so we assume it to be the more probable of the two.

Put more broadly, we mistakenly believe items in larger categories to be more likely to occur than ones in smaller categories. This, dear friends, is known as the Category Size Bias, and it affects all sorts of things.

Let me tell you how you can use it to nudge people to be healthier, to spend more, and be more likely to take action.

Lottery balls

We think that when outcomes are put into relatively large categories, they’re more likely to occur.

In a recent study by Isaac & Brough (2014), 223 people were asked to play a simple lottery, of which there were two versions, each with 15 balls.

The first contained 5 black, 5 grey and 5 white balls.

The second contained 2 black, 11 grey and 2 white balls. You can see here in the second version that the grey is a relatively large category.

Despite the fact that the chance of each ball being plucked from the lottery bowl being equal, people were then asked to estimate the probability of a grey number 8 ball being chosen.

And the answers were crazy! People judged chance of choosing the grey 8 ball to be higher in the lottery with 11 grey balls, than the one with 5 grey balls. Put another way, because there were more grey balls than black or white, they thought that the grey #8 ball was more likely to come up.

The lucky dip

We’ll spend more money on items put in larger categories.

Another experiment from the research involved a glass urn filled with 90 coloured tickets. 81 were blue, and 9 were yellow.

81 students were given one of these blue or yellow tickets, and asked both how likely it was that their ticket would be chosen. They also were asked how much they’d like to bet.

The results showed that those given blue (large category) tickets bet 24% more than those given yellow tickets. They also thought the probability of their ticket being chosen would be higher (24% for blue vs 10% for yellow), despite the fact that each ticket has an equal chance of being chosen (1.67%)! Crazy!

Clearly, this research shows that we feel more confident in choosing something if it’s put within a relatively-large category, and are more likely to take higher risks. Gambling companies take note!

Alphabetting

The more clearly you highlight the difference in category size, the more likely we are to choose something from the larger category.

In a second study, 175 people were asked to guess the probability of a 26-sided alphabet die landing on an ‘A’ (part of the small vowel group) or a ‘T’ (part of the larger consonant group).

The researchers wanted to see whether actively communicating the size of the two groups to the people would have any effect on their judgements. And boy did it!

They tested for 3 levels of information: high, medium and low.

They found that when actively mentioning group size: that T was one of 21 consonants in the alphabet, and A was one of 5 vowels, people were much more likely to think that a T was more likely to be rolled than an A. This is despite the fact that both outcomes have exactly the same chance of happening! Crazy!

The graph below shows you just how much merely communicating category size can affect our decisions.

Computer threat

We are more likely to take action from tasks when they’re in a bigger list, over a smaller list.

One more study from the paper that we’d love to share tested a group’s likelihood to take safety measures against an IT threat. They were given a list of 9 tasks that would protect them, such as “Use a pop up blocker” or “Change your password frequently”.

They were then asked to these 9 tasks into either one of two categories: “identity theft” or “loss of data”. One category would need to end up with 7 items (large), and the other with 2 items (small).

The results of this study are extremely interesting. In the 3 months following the exercise, participants were more likely to engage in those preventative behaviours that they’d classified into a large (vs. small) category of tasks.

And the reason for this greater willingness to act on advice given? People simply felt that the IT threat associated with a large number of preventive behaviours would be more likely to happen than one associated with fewer of them.

In conclusion, this fascinating new research shows us that something as simple as creating and sizing categories has a huge potential to motivate long-lasting behavioural change.

Key takeaways for Decision-Makers

  • Nudging health care This bias has strong implications for policymakers trying to nudge preventative health and safety behaviours. The researchers specifically suggest that when crafting health-related messages, grouping a preventable disease such as lung cancer with a larger number of other potential health risks would positively impact decision-making. This is based on the idea that a reader will believe that they will be more likely to contract a disease from a large group of health risks, with lung cancer being one of them.
  • Aspirational membership schemes and belonging The category size bias provides a credible explanation for why we human beings tend to associate with large groups that are viewed favourably by society. Being part of a large and “desirable” social group can make others believe that we also possess the many qualities of its members. For small businesses, it suggests that forming or being a part of a consortium or large and high quality networking group can dramatically elevate your brand image.
  • Communicating category sizes to nudge effectively Highlighting the differences between the large and small categories is highly likely to enhance the effect of the Category Size Bias. For instance, for software companies, stating that there are 10 features in the premium version versus 4 in the free version will help nudge a decision towards the premium version
  • The many ways to Categorize & Influence…Use colour, size or shape to suggest that items belong to one category or another. As we saw with the lottery balls, just colouring 11 of the 15 balls gray led to people thinking that any one of them is more likely to be chosen.
  • Categorisation leads to more confident, risk-taking decision-makingAs the blue and yellow lottery ticket example shows, we’re likely to take more risks and have a greater level of confidence in choosing something from a larger group, than from a relatively-smaller one. A suggestion of ‘safety in numbers’ seems to play on our minds when making such decisions, even though it’s a total fallacy and has no actual bearing on the chance of success!

If you found this braingem useful or interesting, don’t forget to spread the word. Follow us on Twitter or like our new Facebook page to get all the latest.

What’s a Rich Text element?

The rich text element allows you to create and format headings, paragraphs, blockquotes, images, and video all in one place instead of having to add and format them individually. Just double-click and easily create content.

Static and dynamic content editing

  • Aspirational membership schemes and belonging The category size bias provides a credible explanation for why we human beings tend to associate with large groups that are viewed favourably by society. Being part of a large and “desirable” social group can make others believe that we also possess the many qualities of its members. For small businesses, it suggests that forming or being a part of a consortium or large and high quality networking group can dramatically elevate your brand image.
  • Communicating category sizes to nudge effectively Highlighting the differences between the large and small categories is highly likely to enhance the effect of the Category Size Bias. For instance, for software companies, stating that there are 10 features in the premium version versus 4 in the free version will help nudge a decision towards the premium version

How to customize formatting for each rich text

  1. The findings from this braingem can nudge better healthcare choices, encourage consumption of a given product, and lead to more confident consumer decisions.
  2. We mistakenly believe that items in larger categories have a higher probability of being picked than ones in smaller categories, despite all items having an equal chance of being picked.
  3. We’ll spend or gamble more money on items put in larger categories.
  4. We’re more likely to take action from tasks when they’re in a bigger list, over a smaller list.
  5. We once we put something into a group, we perceive it to adopt all the characteristics of that group. This suggests that small companies should foster alliances with similarly-principled, more established companies.
Back to all Research

What's Cooking?

New Pairing

A Monzo case study on saving refunds by default
A Monzo case study on saving refunds by default
This week

New Nugget

Refund Effect (2024)
Refund Effect (2024)
This week

Improvement

New Nugget 2.0 schematic
New Nugget 2.0 schematic
This week

New Nugget In The Wild

How Experian's CreditLock creates calm with well-designed control
How Experian's CreditLock creates calm with well-designed control
This week

New Cheat Sheet

New "Overcoming Status Quo" Cheat Sheet
New "Overcoming Status Quo" Cheat Sheet
This week

Improvement

Simplified navigation
Simplified navigation
This week

Improvement

After ten years, just who is behind Coglode?
After ten years, just who is behind Coglode?
This week

Feature

Interactive Defaults Tool released
Interactive Defaults Tool released
This week

Coglode Live

Defaults Live video and deck now available
Defaults Live video and deck now available
This week

Cure

Cancelling Amazon Prime - An interactive, behavioural remedy
Cancelling Amazon Prime - An interactive, behavioural remedy
This week

New Nugget In The Wild

How new browser Arc overcomes Status Quo bias with Boosts and Zaps
How new browser Arc overcomes Status Quo bias with Boosts and Zaps
This week

New Cheat Sheet

New "Overcoming Risk Aversion" Cheat Sheet
New "Overcoming Risk Aversion" Cheat Sheet
This week